skip to main content


Title: Building representations of the social world: Children extract patterns from social choices to reason about multi‐group hierarchies
Abstract Research Highlights

Children in our sample extracted patterns from an agent's positive social choices between multiple groups to reason about groups’ relative social standing.

Children used the pattern of an agent's positive social choices to guide their reasoning about which groups were likely to be “leaders” and “helpers” in a fictional town.

The pattern that emerged in an agent's choices of friends shaped children's thinking about groups’ relativesocialbut notphysicalpower.

Children tracked social choices to reason about group‐based hierarchies at the individual level (which groups an agent prefers) and societal level (which groups are privileged).

 
more » « less
NSF-PAR ID:
10395695
Author(s) / Creator(s):
 ;  ;  
Publisher / Repository:
Wiley-Blackwell
Date Published:
Journal Name:
Developmental Science
Volume:
26
Issue:
4
ISSN:
1363-755X
Format(s):
Medium: X
Sponsoring Org:
National Science Foundation
More Like this
  1. Abstract Research Highlights

    A total of 123 4–8‐year‐olds were assigned to advantaged, disadvantaged, and third‐party groups within a hierarchy and were assessed on seven hierarchy‐reinforcing beliefs about the hierarchy.

    Advantaged children were more likely to say the hierarchy was fair, generalizable, and wrong to challenge and to hold intergroup biases favoring advantaged group members.

    With age, advantaged‐ and disadvantaged‐group children held more essentialist beliefs about membership in their own group, but not the behaviors associated with their group.

    Results suggest that advantaged group status can shape how children perceive and respond to the hierarchies they are embedded within.

     
    more » « less
  2. Abstract

    When human adults make decisions (e.g., wearing a seat belt), we often consider the negative consequences that would ensue if our actions were to fail, even if we have never experienced such a failure. Do the same considerations guide our understanding of other people's decisions? In this paper, we investigated whether adults, who have many years of experience making such decisions, and 6‐ and 7‐year‐old children, who have less experience and are demonstrably worse at judging the consequences of their own actions, conceive others' actions as motivated both by reward (how good reaching one's intended goal would be), and by what we call “danger” (how badly one's action could end). In two pre‐registered experiments, we tested whether adults and 6‐ and 7‐year‐old children tailor their predictions and explanations of an agent's action choices to the specific degree of danger and reward entailed by each action. Across four different tasks, we found that children and adults expected others to negatively appraise dangerous situations and minimize the danger of their actions. Children's and adults' judgments varied systematically in accord with both the degree of danger the agent faced and the value the agent placed on the goal state it aimed to achieve. However, children did not calibrate their inferences abouthow muchan agent valued the goal state of a successful action in accord with the degree of danger the action entailed, and adults calibrated these inferences more weakly than inferences concerning the agent's future action choices. These results suggest that from childhood, people use a degree of danger and reward to make quantitative, fine‐grained explanations and predictions about other people's behavior, consistent with computational models on theory of mind that contain continuous representations of other agents' action plans.

     
    more » « less
  3. Abstract Research Highlights

    Older children were more likely to endorse agents whochooseto share over those who do not have a choice.

    Children who were prompted to generate more counterfactuals were more likely to allocate resources to characters with choice.

    Children who generated selfish counterfactuals more positively evaluated agents with choice.

    Comparable to theories suggesting children punish willful transgressors more than accidental transgressors, we propose children also consider free will when making positive moral evaluations.

     
    more » « less
  4. Abstract  
    more » « less
  5. Abstract Research Highlights

    Four studies examined whether 4‐ to 9‐year‐old children's pain perceptions were influenced by sufferers’ wealth status.

    US children attributed more pain to White individuals of low wealth status than those of high wealth status by age seven.

    Chinese children demonstrated an earlier tendency to attribute more pain to the poor (versus the rich) compared to US children.

    Children's wealth‐based pain judgments underlied their tendency to provide healthcare resources to people of low wealth status.

     
    more » « less