Understanding how consumption patterns affect the environment and shape well-being hinges on the rationale that the data collected on what is consumed, who consumes it, and where it is consumed are indeed accurate. To identify these consumption patterns and recommend corresponding policies, researchers and policy makers often rely on national surveys. Studies have explored the accuracy of individual surveys and the level of agreement across surveys of the same type (e.g. household expenditures), but no studies have compared representative national surveys measuring consumption in different ways. This study compares household consumption measured as expenditures and as material consumption (i.e. physical units) to assess how well we currently measure what we consume. We use multiple rigorous, national surveys to estimate meat consumption, household energy use, and private automobile use in the United States, with consumption profiles parsed by affluence, race/ethnicity, and education. Our results indicate that commonly used surveys may not accurately track important aspects of household consumption. For meat consumption, which included 30 consumption profiles detailing the consumption patterns across different demographic characteristics and meat types (e.g. kilograms beef consumed/capita for Caucasians), there is considerable disagreement between data sources for 20 profiles. By contrast, national surveys accurately measure household energy and transport (disagreement for four profiles). Our findings indicate that national surveys more accurately measure consistently tracked, standardized consumables like electricity than irregularly tracked, variable goods such as food. These results cast doubt on studies that use national surveys to draw conclusions about the how the environmental impacts of food, and, potentially, other goods (e.g. manufactured goods) vary across demographic groups. Overcoming this challenge will necessitate new surveys, updating legacy databases, and harnessing breakthroughs in data science.
- PAR ID:
- 10477065
- Publisher / Repository:
- IEEE
- Date Published:
- Journal Name:
- IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics
- ISSN:
- 1077-2626
- Page Range / eLocation ID:
- 1 to 14
- Format(s):
- Medium: X
- Sponsoring Org:
- National Science Foundation
More Like this
-
Abstract -
Abstract Dramatic changes in cranial capacity have characterized human evolution. Important evolutionary hypotheses, such as the spatial packing hypothesis, assert that increases in relative brain size (encephalization) have caused alterations to the modern human skull, resulting in a suite of traits unique among extant primates, including a domed cranial vault, highly flexed cranial base, and retracted facial skeleton. Most prior studies have used fossil or comparative primate data to establish correlations between brain size and cranial form, but the mechanistic basis for how changes in brain size impact the overall shape of the skull resulting in these cranial traits remains obscure and has only rarely been investigated critically. We argue that understanding how changes in human skull morphology could have resulted from increased encephalization requires the direct testing of hypotheses relating to interaction of embryonic development of the bones of the skull and the brain. Fossil and comparative primate data have thoroughly described the patterns of association between brain size and skull morphology. Here we suggest complementing such existing datasets with experiments focused on mechanisms responsible for producing the observed patterns to more thoroughly understand the role of encephalization in shaping the modern human skull.
-
Names for colors vary widely across languages, but color categories are remarkably consistent. Shared mechanisms of color perception help explain consistent partitions of visible light into discrete color vocabularies. But the mappings from colors to words are not identical across languages, which may reflect communicative needs—how often speakers must refer to objects of different color. Here we quantify the communicative needs of colors in 130 different languages by developing an inference algorithm for this problem. We find that communicative needs are not uniform: Some regions of color space exhibit 30-fold greater demand for communication than other regions. The regions of greatest demand correlate with the colors of salient objects, including ripe fruits in primate diets. Our analysis also reveals a hidden diversity in the communicative needs of colors across different languages, which is partly explained by differences in geographic location and the local biogeography of linguistic communities. Accounting for language-specific, nonuniform communicative needs improves predictions for how a language maps colors to words, and how these mappings vary across languages. Our account closes an important gap in the compression theory of color naming, while opening directions to study cross-cultural variation in the need to communicate different colors and its impact on the cultural evolution of color categories.more » « less
-
Abstrakt Jämlikhet, mångfald och inkludering (EDI efter engelska Equity, diversity and inclusivity) är värden som alla forskargrupper och organisationer ska sträva efter. Dessa värden säkrar arbetsvillkor, policies och praktik som inte bara främjar forskningsresultat av hög kvalité men även det vulkanologiska samhällets välbefinnande. I denna artikel reflekterar vi över EDI framsteg inom vulkanologin genom att utvärdera internationella organisationers medlemsdata, sammansättningen av nämnder, fördelning av priser och förstaförfattarskap av artiklar i vulkanologiska tidskrifter. På grund av den otillräckliga tillgången på data fokuserar vi vår analys utifrån diskriminering som kan kopplas till könsidentitet, men visar även att diskriminering på grund av etnicitet, sexuell läggning, religion, fysisk förmåga och socioekonomisk bakgrund förekommer och att om flera av dessa faktorer korsas leder det till ännu värre marginalisering inom det vulkanologiska samhället. Vi delar förslag och råd från andra discipliner om hur individer, forskargrupper och organisationer kan främja, utveckla och implementera nya initiativ som identifierar och tar itu med diskriminering och förbättrar EDI inom vulkanologin. Det finns mycket förbättringspotential om vi alla ser vår roll i att skapa ett jämlikare, och inkluderande vulkanologiskt samhälle med mer mångfald. Att uppnå detta kräver: 1) Medvetenhet: erkännandet av problemet, 2) Engagemang: genom att anta EDI i värdegrunden och utveckla handlingsplaner, uppförandekoder och riktlinjer, 3) Handling med målet att uppnå alla gruppers representation och 4) Reflektion: utveckling genom kritisk självreflektion och viljan att ta itu med brister.more » « less
-
Ruis, Andrew R. ; Lee, Seung B. (Ed.)Coding data—defining concepts and identifying where they occur in data—is a critical aspect of qualitative data analysis, and especially so in quantitative ethnography. Coding is a central process for creating meaning from data, and while much has been written about coding methods and theory, relatively little has been written about what constitutes best practices for fair and valid coding, what justifies those practices, and how to implement them. In this paper, our goal is not to address these issues comprehensively, but to provide guidelines for good coding practice and to highlight some of the issues and key questions that quantitative ethnographers and other researchers should consider when coding data.more » « less